December 23, 2024

Harvard Scientists Make the Case for Female Mice in Neuroscience Research

They discovered that the hormone cycle had a negligible effect on behavior and that differences in behavior in between individual female mice were much greater. Distinctions in behavior were even higher for males than for women, both within and between mice.The results underscore the value of integrating both sexes into mouse research studies, the research study team said.
A 2011 analysis found that there were over 5 times as many single-sex neuroscience research studies of male mice than of female mice. Maybe the greatest reason for excluding female mice, Datta stated, stems from a prevalent presumption that their behavior is broadly affected by cyclic variations in hormonal agents such as estrogen and progesterone– the rodent version of a menstrual cycle, understood as the estrous cycle. In practice, the test involved placing a mouse in a 5-gallon Home Depot bucket for 20 minutes and using a camera to tape-record the mouses movements and habits in 3D as it freely explored the space.

Mice have actually long been a main part of neuroscience research study, providing a flexible model that researchers can control and study to read more about the detailed inner functions of the brain. Historically, researchers have actually favored male mice over female mice in experiments, in part due to concern that the hormonal agent cycle in females causes behavioral variation that could toss off results.
New research from Harvard Medical School (HMS) obstacles this idea and suggests that for many experiments, the concern may not be warranted.

Male mice have actually been preferred over female mice in neuroscience research due to the concern that the hormonal agent cycle in females might result in behavioral variations that could affect the precision of the results. Brand-new research from Harvard Medical School suggests that this concern might not be warranted in many experiments.
Research study findings expose that female mice have more steady exploratory habits than male mice.

New research suggests that female mice reveal more steady exploratory behavior than male mice, despite hormone cycles
The outcomes challenge a long-held presumption that hormonal agents have a broad impact on behavior in female mice, making them less appropriate for research study
The findings make a strong scientific case for increasing the addition of female mice in neuroscience and other experiments

According to the study, female mice, despite ongoing hormone variations, show exploratory habits that is more steady than that of their male peers. The study results were released on March 7 in the journal Current Biology.
Utilizing a pressure of mice commonly studied in laboratory settings, the researchers examined how the animals acted as they easily explored an open area. They found that the hormone cycle had a negligible result on habits and that distinctions in behavior in between individual female mice were much greater. Differences in habits were even higher for males than for females, both within and between mice.The results highlight the importance of incorporating both sexes into mouse research studies, the research team stated.
” I think this is truly powerful evidence that if youre studying naturalistic, spontaneous exploratory behavior, you need to include both sexes in your experiments– and it causes the argument that in this setting, if you can only choose one sex to deal with, you ought to really be working on women,” said Sandeep Robert Datta, professor of neurobiology in the Blavatnik Institute at HMS, who co-led the research study with Rebecca Shansky of Northeastern University.
From rodents to human beings: A history of predisposition
As neuroscientists make every effort to much better comprehend the human brain, they routinely rely on the mouse, which Datta considers “the flagship vertebrate design for comprehending how the brain works.”
This is because mouse and human brains share a significant quantity of structural organization and genetic details, so scientists can easily control the mouse genome to address specific experimental concerns and to build designs of human illness.
” Much of what we comprehend about the relationship in between genes and neural circuits, and between neural activity and habits, comes from standard research in the mouse, and mouse models are likely going to be really main tools in our battle versus a broad range of neurological and mental illness,” Datta stated.
For more than 50 years, researchers have preferentially used male mice in experiments, and nowhere has this practice been more prominent than in neuroscience. A 2011 analysis found that there were over 5 times as lots of single-sex neuroscience research studies of male mice than of female mice. In time, this practice has resulted in a poorer understanding of the female brain, most likely contributing to the misdiagnosis of neurological and mental conditions in women, as well as the development of drugs that have more adverse effects for females– problems outlined by Shansky in a 2021 viewpoint in Nature Neuroscience.
The variation in sex representation typical in animal research has actually likewise been traditionally mirrored in research study involving human topics.
” This bias starts in standard science, however the repercussions are rolled into drug advancement, and cause bias in drugs being produced, and how drugs are suited for the various sexes,” stated lead author Dana Levy, a research fellow in neurobiology at HMS. For instance, Levy kept in mind that conditions such as anxiety, stress and anxiety, and pain are known to manifest in a different way in female mice and females than in the male mice that are more typically utilized in early-stage drug screening.
To address the problem of sex predisposition in scientific research, the National Institutes of Health released a policy in 2016 requiring researchers to include female and male subjects and samples in experiments. Follow-up studies that look throughout clinical disciplines and examine neuroscience specifically show that development has actually been sluggish.
The reasons for such a long-standing predisposition in neuroscience are made complex, Datta stated: “Part of it is just plain old sexism, and part of it is conservatism in the sense that people have actually studied male mice for so long that they do not want to make a change.”
Perhaps the greatest factor for excluding female mice, Datta stated, stems from an extensive assumption that their behavior is broadly affected by cyclic variations in hormonal agents such as estrogen and progesterone– the rodent variation of a menstrual cycle, understood as the estrous cycle. According to Datta and Levy, estrous status is understood to have a strong effect on specific social and sexual behaviors in mice. However, data on the impact of estrous status in other behavioral contexts have actually been combined, resulting in what Datta calls “an authentic argument in the literature.”
” We wanted to determine how much the estrous cycle appeared to affect basic patterns of expedition,” Datta stated. “Our concern was whether these ongoing modifications in the hormone state of the mouse impact other neural circuits in such a way thats puzzling for scientists.”
” We presumed, like everybody else, that adding women was simply going to complicate our experiments,” Levy included, “And so we said, why not evaluate this.”.
Checking assumptions.
The scientists studied genetically similar males and females from a typical pressure of laboratory mouse in a circular open field– a basic lab setup for behavioral neuroscience experiments. In practice, the test involved putting a mouse in a 5-gallon Home Depot bucket for 20 minutes and using an electronic camera to tape-record the mouses movements and behaviors in 3D as it freely explored the space. The researchers swabbed each female mouse to determine its estrous status and repeated the bucket test with the very same individual numerous times.
The team examined the videos with MoSeq, an expert system innovation formerly developed by the laboratory. The technology utilizes machine learning algorithms to break down a mouses movements into around 50 different “syllables,” or parts of body language: short, single motions such as rearing up, stopping briefly, stepping, or turning. With MoSeq, the scientists collected extensive, high-resolution information about the structure and pattern of mouse behavior throughout each session.
The scientists discovered that estrous status had very little result on exploratory habits in female mice. Instead, patterns of behavior tended to differ far more across female mice than they did throughout the estrous cycle.
” If you offer me any random video from our pile, I can inform you which mouse it is. Thats how embellished the pattern of habits is,” Datta said, which recommends that in behavioral studies, “a dominant element of variation in the information is the truth that individuals have subtly different life histories.”.
When the researchers compared female and male mice, they discovered something that amazed them: Males also exhibited uniqueness of behavior, but they had more behavioral variation within a single mouse and in between mice than women.
” People have been making this presumption that we can use male mice to reliably make contrasts within and across experiments, but our information recommend that female mice are more stable in regards to habits despite the fact that they have the estrous cycle,” Datta said.
A case for change.
Scientists typically agree that including female mice is essential from a fairness viewpoint, Datta kept in mind, yet some have stayed concerned that it might complicate their research study. For him, the brand-new findings make a strong scientific case for using female mice in experiments.
” The truth that female behavior is more trusted recommends that consisting of women might in fact decrease the general irregularity in your data under numerous situations,” Datta said.
Based on their findings, researchers in the Datta laboratory have actually currently switched from male mice to combined groups or female mice in their other experiments that involve circular, open-field testing.
Datta cautioned that the research study looks at just one mouse strain in one lab setup, therefore the results can not be generalized to other stress and setups without additional testing. Nevertheless, he kept in mind that the stress and setup are commonly utilized in neuroscience research study, consisting of in early-stage drug development to evaluate how a prospective drug impacts mouse mobility.
Datta said that the findings “must encourage folks who have an interest in drug development in this context to consist of both sexes in their analysis.”.
Now, Datta and Levy are interested in checking out how internal states beyond hormone status, such as cravings, thirst, disease, and discomfort, affect exploratory habits in mice.
” The concern is, who wins in this tug-of-war in between your current internal state and your individual identity,” Levy explained.
They likewise desire to dive deeper into the neural basis of the uniqueness of mouse habits that they saw in the study.
” I was stunned by just how much stable variation between individuals we were observing– its like these mice actually are individuals,” Datta stated. “Were used to thinking about lab mice as interchangeable widgets, but theyre not at all. What is managing these customized patterns of habits?”.
” We wish to comprehend the mechanisms of uniqueness: how variability between individuals happens, how it impacts behavior, what can alter it, and what brain areas support it,” Levy included.
To this end, the Datta laboratory is analyzing mouse habits from birth up until death to understand how personalized patterns of habits emerge and crystallize during development, and how they change throughout life.
The researchers also hope that their work will open the door for more rigorous, quantitative research study on whether and how the estrous cycle affects mouse habits in other contexts, such as finishing complicated jobs.
” This is an extremely fascinating example of how assumptions that impact the method that we carry out and develop our science are sometimes simply presumptions– and it is necessary to directly evaluate them, because in some cases theyre not real,” Levy stated.
Recommendation: “Mouse spontaneous behavior shows individual variation instead of estrous state” by Dana Rubi Levy, Nigel Hunter, Sherry Lin, Emma Marie Robinson, Winthrop Gillis, Eli Benjamin Conlin, Rockwell Anyoha, Rebecca M. Shansky and Sandeep Robert Datta, 7 March 2023, Current Biology.DOI: 10.1016/ j.cub.2023.02.035.
Extra authors consist of Nigel Hunter, Sherry Lin, Emma Robinson, Winthrop Gillis, Eli Conlin, and Rockwell Anyoha of HMS.
The research was supported by the NIH (U19NS113201; RF1AG073625; R01NS114020), the Brain Research Foundation, the Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain, the Simons Collaboration for Plasticity in the Aging Brain, the Human Frontier Science Program, and the Zuckerman STEM Leadership Program.
Datta is on the scientific boards of advisers of Neumora, Inc., and Gilgamesh Pharmaceuticals, which have accredited the MoSeq technology.