December 23, 2024

Brain’s Limitations Uncovered: Scientists Debunk Long-Held Beliefs About Neural Rewiring

Recent research study by Professors Tamar Makin and John Krakauer challenges the commonly held belief that the brain can rewire itself to make up for sensory losses like blindness or the effects of a stroke. Their analysis of seminal studies exposes that the brain does not create brand-new functions in previously unrelated locations but rather enhances its pre-existing architecture through knowing and repetition. This understanding is essential for setting reasonable expectations in rehabilitation and valuing the effort behind recovery stories.
Contrary to common belief, the brain does not have the capability to rewire itself to make up for loss of sight, amputations, or stroke-related damage, according to scientists from the University of Cambridge and Johns Hopkins University.
In a current paper published in eLife, Professors Tamar Makin (Cambridge) and John Krakauer (Johns Hopkins) argue that the idea that the brain, in action to injury or deficit, can rearrange itself and repurpose specific areas for new functions, is fundamentally flawed– despite being typically pointed out in scientific textbooks. Instead, they argue that what is happening is merely the brain being trained to utilize currently existing, but hidden, abilities.
Mistaken Beliefs of Brain Plasticity
Among the most typical examples provided is where a person loses their sight– or is born blind– and the visual cortex, formerly specialized in processing vision, is rewired to process noises, permitting the private to use a form of echolocation to navigate a messy space. Another common example is of individuals who have actually had a stroke and are at first unable to move their limbs repurposing other areas of the brain to allow them to restore control.

Krakauer, Director of the Center for the Study of Motor Learning and Brain Repair at Johns Hopkins University, said: “The idea that our brain has a fantastic capability to rewire and reorganize itself is an appealing one. It offers us hope and fascination, particularly when we hear remarkable stories of blind individuals establishing almost superhuman echolocation abilities, for instance, or stroke survivors miraculously gaining back motor capabilities they believed they d lost.
” This idea goes beyond basic adaptation, or plasticity– it suggests a wholesale repurposing of brain regions. While these stories might well be real, the description of what is occurring is, in truth, incorrect.”
Reassessing Seminal Studies
In their post, Makin and Krakauer take a look at ten critical studies that purport to show the brains capability to restructure. They argue, nevertheless, that while the studies do indeed reveal the brains ability to adjust to change, it is not producing brand-new functions in formerly unrelated locations– rather its making use of hidden capacities that have actually existed considering that birth.
One of the studies– research carried out in the 1980s by Professor Michael Merzenich at University of California, San Francisco– looked at what takes place when a hand loses a finger. The hand has a specific representation in the brain, with each finger appearing to map onto a specific brain area. Remove the forefinger, and the area of the brain formerly designated to this finger is reallocated to processing signals from neighbouring fingers, argued Merzenich– simply put, the brain has actually rewired itself in response to changes in sensory input.
Not so, states Makin, whose own research supplies an alternative description.
Challenging the Rewiring Theory
In a research study published in 2022, Makin used a nerve blocker to momentarily mimic the impact of amputation of the forefinger in her subjects. She showed that even before amputation, signals from surrounding fingers mapped onto the brain area accountable for the forefinger– simply put, while this brain region might have been mostly responsible for procedure signals from the forefinger, it was not specifically so. All that takes place following amputation is that existing signals from the other fingers are dialed up in this brain area.
Makin, from the Medical Research Council (MRC) Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit at the University of Cambridge, stated: “The brains ability to adapt to injury isnt about commandeering new brain regions for completely different purposes. These regions dont start processing entirely new kinds of information. Information about the other fingers was offered in the analyzed brain area even before the amputation, its just that in the original research studies, the researchers didnt pay much notification to it because it was weaker than for the finger about to be amputated.”
Proof from Congenitally Deaf Cats
Another compelling counterexample to the reorganization argument is seen in a research study of congenitally deaf cats, whose auditory cortex– the location of the brain that processes sound– appears to be repurposed to process vision. When they are fitted with a cochlear implant, this brain region instantly starts processing sound once again, suggesting that the brain had not, in fact, rewired.
Taking a look at other studies, Makin and Krakauer found no compelling evidence that the visual cortex of people that were born blind or the unscathed cortex of stroke survivors ever established an unique practical capability that did not otherwise exist..
Understanding True Brain Plasticity.
Makin and Krakauer do not dismiss the stories of blind people being able to navigate simply based on hearing, or people who have experienced a stroke restore their motor functions. They argue rather that instead of completely repurposing regions for new tasks, the brain is enhancing or customizing its pre-existing architecture– and it is doing this through repeating and learning.
Comprehending the real nature and limitations of brain plasticity is vital, both for setting realistic expectations for clients and for assisting clinical specialists in their rehabilitative techniques, they argue.
Makin included: “This knowing procedure is a testament to the brains impressive– but constrained– capacity for plasticity. The idea of quickly opening hidden brain potentials or tapping into large unused reserves is more wishful thinking than truth.
” So numerous times, the brains capability to rewire has been referred to as incredible– but were scientists, we do not think in magic. These remarkable habits that we see are rooted in effort, repeating, and training, not the magical reassignment of the brains resources.”.
Reference: “Against cortical reorganisation” by Tamar R Makin and John W Krakauer, 21 November 2023, eLife.DOI: doi:10.7554/ eLife.84716.

Current research by Professors Tamar Makin and John Krakauer challenges the commonly held belief that the brain can rewire itself to compensate for sensory losses like blindness or the effects of a stroke. The hand has a specific representation in the brain, with each finger appearing to map onto a specific brain region. Get rid of the forefinger, and the area of the brain formerly assigned to this finger is reallocated to processing signals from neighbouring fingers, argued Merzenich– in other words, the brain has actually rewired itself in reaction to changes in sensory input.
She showed that even before amputation, signals from neighboring fingers mapped onto the brain region accountable for the forefinger– in other words, while this brain region might have been mostly responsible for process signals from the forefinger, it was not exclusively so. Makin, from the Medical Research Council (MRC) Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit at the University of Cambridge, said: “The brains ability to adjust to injury isnt about commandeering brand-new brain areas for entirely different purposes.