May 15, 2024

Scientists Discover a Way To Manipulate the Brain’s Perception of Time

Their study manipulated neural activity patterns in rats, deforming their understanding of time duration. From Aristotles musings on the nature of time to Einsteins theory of relativity, mankind has long contemplated: how do we perceive and understand time? The theory of relativity presumes that time can stretch and contract, a phenomenon understood as time dilation. Simply as the cosmos contorts time, our neural circuits can stretch and compress our subjective experience of time.” Temperature then gave us a knob with which to stretch or contract neural activity in time, so we applied this control in the context of behavior,” says Filipe Rodrigues, another lead author in the research study.

Champalimaud Researchs Learning Lab has actually provided significant evidence of how the brains internal clockwork guides behavior. Their research study controlled neural activity patterns in rats, deforming their understanding of time duration. This research not just challenges traditional understanding of time measurement but likewise has possible therapeutic implications for diseases like Parkinsons and Huntingtons, and for robotics and finding out algorithms.
The Learning Lab at Champalimaud Research has actually discovered a way to manipulate the brains perception of time by controlling neural activity in rats. Their research study, which has prospective applications in treating illness like Parkinsons and Huntingtons, might also affect the fields of robotics and finding out algorithms.
From Aristotles musings on the nature of time to Einsteins theory of relativity, mankind has long considered: how do we understand and view time? Just as the cosmos deforms time, our neural circuits can extend and compress our subjective experience of time.
In brand-new work from Champalimaud Researchs Learning Lab released in the journal Nature Neuroscience, scientists artificially slowed down, or accelerated, patterns of neural activity in rats, deforming their judgment of time duration and supplying the most engaging causal evidence so far for how the brains inner clockwork guides habits.

In contrast to the more familiar circadian clocks that govern our 24-hour biological rhythms and form our every day lives, from sleep-wake cycles to metabolic process, much less is learnt about how the body procedures time on the scale of seconds to minutes. The study focused specifically on this seconds-to-minutes timescale at which much of our behavior unfolds, whether youre waiting at a red light or serving a tennis ball.
The Population Clock Hypothesis
Unlike the exact ticking of a computer systems central clock, our brains keep a decentralized and flexible sense of time, believed to be shaped by the dynamics of neuronal networks distributed throughout the brain. In this “population clock” hypothesis, our brains keep time by depending on constant patterns of activity progressing in groups of neurons during habits.
Joe Paton, the research studys senior author, likens this to dropping a stone into a pond. “Each time a stone is dropped, it produces ripples that radiate outward on the surface area in a repeatable pattern. By examining the patterns and positions of these ripples, one can deduce when and where the stone was dropped into the water.”
Researchers artificially decreased, or sped up, patterns of neural activity in rats, warping their judgment of time period. Credit: Created by Hedi Young with the assistance of Stable Diffusion
” Just as the speed at which the ripples move can differ, the pace at which these activity patterns progress in neural populations can also shift. Our laboratory was one of the first to show a tight connection between how quick or slow these neural ripples progress and time-dependent choices.”
The researchers trained rats to identify in between different periods of time. They discovered that activity in the striatum, a deep brain region, follows predictable patterns that alter at various speeds: when animals report a given time period as longer, the activity evolves faster, and when they report it as shorter, the activity progresses more gradually.
However, connection does not imply causation. “We wanted to check whether variability in the speed of striatal population characteristics simply correlates with or straight manages timing habits. To do that, we needed a way to experimentally control these dynamics as animals reported timing judgments.”
Deciphering Time with Temperature
” Never discard old tools,” smiles Tiago Monteiro, one of the research studys lead authors. To develop causation, the team turned to an old-school technique in the neuroscientists tool kit: temperature level. “Temperature has actually been utilized in previous studies to control the temporal characteristics of behaviors, such as bird song. Cooling a particular brain region slows down the song, while warming speeds it up, without altering its structure. Its comparable to altering the tempo of a musical piece without affecting the notes themselves. We believed temperature level might be perfect as it would possibly allow us to alter the speed of neural dynamics without interrupting its pattern.”
To check this tool in rats, they developed a custom-made thermoelectric device to warm or cool the striatum focally, while simultaneously recording neural activity. They found that undoubtedly cooling dilated the pattern of activity, while warming contracted it, without perturbing the pattern itself.
” Temperature then provided us a knob with which to stretch or contract neural activity in time, so we applied this adjustment in the context of habits,” states Filipe Rodrigues, another lead author in the study. “We trained animals to report whether the period between two tones was much shorter or longer than 1.5 seconds. They were more likely to state a provided interval was short when we cooled the striatum. When we warmed it, they were most likely to say that it was long.” Warming up the striatum sped up striatal population dynamics, comparable to accelerating the motion of a clocks hands, causing the rats to evaluate a given time interval as being longer than it actually was.
2 Brain Systems for Motor Control
” Surprisingly,” includes Paton, “even though the striatum collaborates motor control, slowing down or speeding up its patterns of activity does not correspondingly slow down or speed up the animals motions in the task. This got us thinking more deeply about the nature of habits control in basic. Even the easiest organisms face 2 fundamental challenges when it comes to managing movement.
The groups findings indicate that the striatum is crucial for resolving the first difficulty– determining what to do and when– while the 2nd obstacle of how to control the continuous motion is left to other brain structures. In a different study, the team is now exploring the cerebellum, which houses majority of the brains neurons, and is connected with continuous, moment-by-moment execution of our actions. “Interestingly,” Paton reveals, “our initial data shows that using temperature manipulations to the cerebellum, unlike the striatum, does affect constant motion control.”
As Paton mentions, “You can see this department of labor between the two brain systems in movement conditions like Parkinsons and cerebellar ataxia.”
Parkinsons, an illness that affects the striatum, typically hinders clients capability to self-initiate motor plans, such as strolling. These hints likely engage other brain regions, such as the cerebellum and cortex, which are still undamaged and can effectively manage constant movement.
Ramifications and Future Directions
By supplying brand-new insights into the causal relationship between neural activity and timing function, the teams outcomes may advance the development of novel healing targets for debilitating illness such as Parkinsons and Huntingtons, which involve time-related symptoms and jeopardized striatum. Additionally, by highlighting a more specific function for the striatum in discrete, rather than continuous, motor control, the outcomes might likewise affect algorithmic structures used in robotics and knowing.
” Ironically, for a paper about time, this study was years in the making,” says Monteiro. “But theres plenty more mystery to decipher. What brain circuits produce these timekeeping ripples of activity in the first place? What calculations, aside from keeping time, might such ripples perform? How do they help us adjust and react smartly to our environment? To respond to these questions, were going to require more of something weve been studying … time.”.
Reference: “Using temperature to evaluate the neural basis of a time-based choice” by Tiago Monteiro, Filipe S. Rodrigues, Margarida Pexirra, Bruno F. Cruz, Ana I. Gonçalves, Pavel E. Rueda-Orozco and Joseph J. Paton, 13 July 2023, Nature Neuroscience.DOI: 10.1038/ s41593-023-01378-5.